This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
The popularity of smartphones is undeniable in nearly all facets of society. Despite the many benefits attributed to the technology, concern has grown over the potential for excessive smartphone use to become problematic in nature. Due to the growing concerns surrounding the recognized and unrecognized implications of smartphone use, great efforts have been made through research to evaluate, label and identify problematic smartphone use mostly through the development and administration of scales assessing the behavior. This study examines 78 existing validated scales that have been developed over the past 13 years to measure, identify or characterize excessive or problematic smartphone use by evaluating their theoretical foundations and their psychometric properties. Our review determined that, despite an abundance of self-report scales examining the construct, many published scales lack sufficient internal consistency and test-retest reliability. Additionally, there is a lack of research supporting the theoretical foundation of many of the scales evaluated. Future research is needed to better characterize problematic smartphone use so that assessment tools can be more efficiently developed to evaluate the behavior in order to avoid the excessive publication of seemingly redundant assessment tools.
Keywords: mobile phone, smartphone, problematic use, addiction, assessmentSmartphone ownership has become increasingly more prevalent over the past decade since Apple’s first iPhone smartphone device was launched in 2007 (Apple Inc, 2007). In 2018, the Consumer Technology Association (CTA) revealed that smartphones were owned in 87% of United States homes and predicted that smartphone ownership could reach household TV ownership rates (96%) within 5 years (Twice Staff, 2018). However, in the fields of psychology and cognition, it is not the mere ownership of the technological devices that is causing increased concern. It is, instead, the potential for dysfunction associated with smartphone use that is leading researchers to stress the importance of investigating the behavior. Therefore, the purpose of this paper is 3-fold. First, we review literature examining psychological and behavioral dysfunctions related to smartphone use as well as probe the potential role problematic smartphone usage may occupy within the realm of addiction research. Second, we present an exhaustive review of assessment scales that measure problematic smartphone or mobile phone use including an overview of reliability (i.e., internal consistency and test-retest reliability) and criterion-related validity by each scale. Third, we will provide specific recommendations for moving the field forward including furthering research in order to standardize conceptualization of the behavior.
It is important to note that a standard cut-off point to determine when smartphone use becomes problematic has yet to be established. Due to insufficient research investigating problematic smartphone use in order to effectively and consistently characterize it, it is currently unclear whether “problematic use” ought to be defined by use quantity, patterns of use, or by the negative consequences of the use. Billieux (2012) conducted a frequently cited literature review of dysfunctional mobile phone use and defined the problematic use of mobile phones as “an inability to regulate one’s use of the mobile phone, which eventually involves negative consequences in daily life” (pg. 1). Numerous research studies indicating that smartphone use is related to various facets of dysfunction support Billieux’s (2012) conceptualization of problematic use being contingent upon negative consequences associated with the use. Evidence has accumulated showing strong links between smartphone use and social, interpersonal, mental health, cognition and academic dysfunction, suggesting that smartphone use can result in significant negative consequences for some individuals (see review, Billieux, 2012).
For example, although smartphones provide unique opportunities for social interaction, Scott et al. (2016) found that problematic attachment to technology such as smartphone devices was associated with lowered social skills, emotional intelligence and empathy, as well as increased conflict with others. Additionally, Laramie (2007) identified social anxiety and loneliness as being associated with heavy use of and reliance upon mobile phones, suggesting smartphone overuse may result in interpersonal dysfunction. Relatedly, self-reported subjective smartphone addiction has been shown to be negatively correlated with psychological well-being (Kumcagiz and Gündüz, 2016). Several studies have revealed evidence that low self-esteem (Bianchi and Phillips, 2005; Hong et al., 2012). and depression and anxiety (De-Sola et al., 2017b; Elhai et al., 2017; Matar Boumosleh and Jaalouk, 2017) are associated with problematic smartphone use, especially in populations of adolescents and young adults. The results of these studies present rationale for a justified concern surrounding potential negative psychological consequences of smartphone overuse.
Similarly, concern has grown over the potential negative impacts smartphone use might have on users’ behavior and cognitive abilities. Research has shown that problematic smartphone use is related to impulsivity (Contractor et al., 2017; De-Sola et al., 2017b; Hadar et al., 2017), impaired attention (Roberts et al., 2015; Hadar et al., 2017), and compromised inhibitory control (Chen et al., 2016). These associated cognitive deficits have spurred researchers to investigate the potential for dysfunction in academic performance, as well. Smartphone use has been shown to negatively correlate with academic progress and success (Alosaimi et al., 2016; Hawi and Samaha, 2016; Samaha and Hawi, 2016). Findings from these studies suggest the cognitive dysfunction associated with smartphone overuse may result in real-world consequences for some individuals.
To reiterate, despite research efforts characterizing associated dysfunction, a standardized conceptualization of problematic smartphone use has yet to be established in the field. However, the previously described areas of dysfunction (e.g., social, interpersonal, mental health, cognition, and academia) found to be associated with smartphone use support Billieux’s (2012) conceptualization of problematic smartphone use being contingent upon negative consequences associated with the use. As such, many assessment tools for problematic use tap into these types of negative life consequences as they are likely to identify individuals for which excessive smartphone use is especially harmful.
The American Psychiatric Association (APA) broadly defines addiction as “a complex condition, a brain disease that is manifested by compulsive substance use despite harmful consequences” (pg. 1; American Psychiatric Association, 2017). In this definition, the use of substances is a requirement of the condition in that, to be “addicted,” one must have a substance to which to be addicted. But, what about behavioral addictions? Both the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11; World Health Organization [WHO], 2018) have grouped behavioral addictions within their respective substance dependence categories. Re-categorization of addictions was seen in the DSM-5 resulting in gambling disorder being recognized as a non-substance-related addictive disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Additionally, Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) is included in the DSM-5 as a condition for further study (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Finally, both gambling disorder and gaming disorder are grouped together in the ICD-11 (World Health Organization [WHO], 2018), suggesting behavioral addictions share some common ground with substance use disorders (SUD).
Despite this conceptual similarity, Billieux et al. (2015) argue that, while addictive behaviors like problematic smartphone use is associated with several types of associated dysfunction, research in this arena is inconsistent in documenting significant behavioral and neurobiological similarities and correlates with more widely recognized substance addictions. For example, there are many features of substance addiction that do not appear to be present when considering excessive smartphone use. Very little research has documented the presence of loss of control (i.e., trouble consciously limiting one’s smartphone use), tolerance (i.e., increasing smartphone use to achieve satisfaction), and withdrawal (i.e., negative symptoms that occur after smartphone use discontinuation; Billieux et al., 2015). Also, dependence symptoms such as tolerance and withdrawal, theoretically based in physiological adaptation to increasing amounts of a drug, are often absent in behavioral addictions. In their review for IGD, Kaptsis et al. (2016) did not find consistent answers to questions inquiring about withdrawal symptoms, such as effects on mood (i.e., feeling “irritable,” “dissatisfied,” or “moody” when unable to play a game) for IGD. Similarly, physiological and neurobiological adaptations to increasing amounts of smartphone use have yet to be documented, suggesting researchers may need to use other criterion to define problematic smartphone use. Some researchers have argued that “borrowing” such criteria from more recognized addictive behaviors, like substance abuse or problematic gambling, might not fit for certain problematic or excessive behaviors (Starcevic, 2016). Thus, although sharing common ground, problematic smartphone use may substantially differ from substance addiction in regards to loss of control, tolerance, and withdrawal.
Some other criteria for addiction map on better. Aforementioned associated life dysfunction is becoming increasingly documented, meaning the problematic use of smartphone devices has real-world negative consequences for some individuals. Compulsive use has been documented: Parasuraman et al. (2017) found that over 50% of participants would not quit using their smartphones even though their daily lifestyles were being negatively affected by their excessive use. This irresistible impulse to use one’s smartphone despite wanting to stop is reminiscent of individuals with SUDs, in which the drive to use drugs overrides other executive control processes. Six symptom criteria were even proposed to diagnose smartphone “addiction” and related functional impairment, which were based on guidelines for SUDs and IGD. Lin et al. (2016) dropped tolerance from their final criterion, due to low diagnostic accuracy. However, they included withdrawal, as subjects who used their smartphones excessively enough to be considered “addicted” displayed feelings of dysphoria, anxiety, and/or irritability after a period without their smartphones.
Dependency appears, to some extent, in excess smartphone users, although again this is based on subjective self-report. In a study conducted by Parasuraman et al. (2017) analyzing smartphone use behavior, almost 75% of smartphone users reported feeling dependent upon smartphone devices and 58% of users felt as though they were “unable to withstand” not having their smartphone with them. Additionally, over 70% of participants indicated that they use their smartphone longer than they intended. Similarly, results from a research study released by The Sun newspaper in March of 2013 indicated that one in ten college students say that they are “addicted” to their smartphones (Hope, 2013). Upon surveying 2,000 college students, 85% of the students endorsed the question about constantly checking their smartphones to figure out what time it is and 75% of the students responded that they sleep with their smartphones lying beside them. These data indicate, when used excessively, smartphones can become problematic and users report feeling as though they have an addiction to them.
Laws have even been enacted in many states to combat problematic use. Phone use while driving a vehicle has become a major concern and it has been shown that it is the anticipation of incoming calls, messages and notifications that directly correlates with greater in-vehicle phone use (O’Connor et al., 2013, 2017). Additionally, recently, the city of Honolulu, Hawaii has even gone so far as to enact a law making it illegal for pedestrians to use their phones when crossing a street or highway (Honolulu, Hawaii, Ordinance 17-39, Bill 6, 2017) due to the significant increase in pedestrian fatalities in the city partially attributed to smartphone distraction (Ellis, 2017). Thus, more and more individuals are using their smartphones in risky and physically hazardous situations. This is conceptually similar to some more recognized addiction criteria for SUDs in DSM-5.
As reviewed, problematic smartphone use shares some conceptual similarities with more typically recognized addictions, including excessive use, failure of impulse control, feelings of dependency, use in risky and/or physically hazardous situations, and potential for negative affect when not using one’s smartphone. The term “addiction” is typically characterized by these criterion, but the question of whether “behavioral addictions” must contain all of these same criterion to be considered a true “addiction” is still under debate.
Starcevic (2013) suggested behavioral addictions are characterized by salient behaviors which promote craving and neglect of other life activity, loss of control, tolerance and withdrawal manifestations, and negative consequences from overuse. Gambling disorder, considered an impulse control disorder in the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), is now characterized and grouped with SUDs in the most recent DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) in a new category of psychopathology entitled “Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders.” This transition was the result of a wide body of research demonstrating clinical, phenomenological, genetic, neurobiological, and other similarities between gambling disorder and SUDs (Potenza, 2014). While gambling disorder is currently the only representative member of the “Non-Substance-Related Disorders” subsection, this transition was an important shift for the recognition of “behavioral addictions” more broadly. Many researchers now advocate for the similar recognition of problematic smartphone use (e.g., Potenza et al., 2018).
Support for recognition of problematic smartphone use has also been motivated by the growing body of research literature on Internet addiction seen since the late 1990s. Kimberly Young is considered to be the “founder” of the concept of Internet addiction due to her publication of a case study in 1996 involving a 43-year-old female with no addiction or psychiatric history who abused the Internet causing significant impairment (Young, 1996). This led to her development and validation of the Young Internet Addiction Scale (Y-Scale; Young, 1998) assessing self-reported preoccupation with the Internet, need to use the Internet with increasing amounts of time, unsuccessful efforts to stop use, restlessness associated with decreased use, longer than intended use, associated life impairments, concealment of involvement, and use of the Internet to relieve a dysphoric mood. The scale’s items were derived from the DSM-IV’s criteria for Pathological Gambling (American Psychiatric Association, 1995) due to her conceptualization of the behavior as being similar to other impulse-control disorders. It seems likely that the development of this scale and the subsequent research that has been conducted on Internet addiction have greatly influenced the investigation of problematic smartphone use as a similar disorder.
In light of growing concerns surrounding the known and unknown implications of smartphone use as well as these recent changes in the conceptualization of non-substance-related addictions, great efforts have been made through research to identify, label and evaluate problematic smartphone use mostly through the development and administration of scales measuring and characterizing the behavior. Researchers within the past 13 years have set out to develop assessment tools based upon varying diagnostic criteria for officially recognized disorders and addictions such as SUDs and gambling disorder as well as unofficial criteria associated with Internet addiction. The aim of the present review is to examine existing validated scales that have been developed to measure, identify or characterize problematic smartphone use by evaluating their theoretical foundations and their psychometric properties.
All studies (published between January 1994 and May 2019) validating standardized measures of varying forms of problematic smartphone use were identified by searching two databases (PsycINFO and MEDLINE Complete) through EBSCOhost. The date range was decided upon after conducting a preemptive literature search utilizing the search terms listed in Appendix A and concluding that the earliest study was published in 1994 (Clifford et al., 1994). For the EBSCOhost literature collection, language was limited to English. Further studies, including those in other languages, were identified by reviewing the bibliographies of relevant studies and reviews.
Due to inconsistencies in the field regarding the conceptualization of the technology being used and the use of the technology, various terms were used in order to ensure that all relevant studies would be identified and reviewed. In addition to searching for studies identifying problematic use of smartphones, terms such as “smart phone,” “cellular phone,” “cell phone,” “mobile device,” and “mobile phone” were used. Additionally, because of the conceptualization of the problematic use has also been shown to be inconsistently described in research studies, terms such as “dependence,” “dependency,” “overuse,” “nomophobia,” “attachment,” and “compulsive” were used during the literature collection process. Finally, terms such as “questionnaire,” “scale,” “inventory,” measurement,” and “validation” were used to ensure all studies validating measurement scales were identified. The full search strategy is presented in Appendix A.
Scales were selected for inclusion if: (a) their development and validation were investigated in the identified study, or (b) they were described in the methods section of a research study as being used to identify or evaluate the behavior. Scales were excluded from the systematic review when they were used to measure behavior not specific to smartphone or cellular phone problematic use.
Once a measurement scale was identified through the review of a study, a structured process was used to extract data on the scale (title, abbreviation, and the author(s) of original development/validation study), items (total number, format, and scale range), sample and norms (validation study participant count and descriptions), reliability (internal consistency and temporal stability), validity (content domains and criterion-related validity), and construct being measured. If a scale was mentioned in a research study as being used to measure the behavior, the study used to validate the scale and discuss its development was found in the study’s references and used to extract these data.
Format of the scale items was identified and described as either Likert scale (range of potential responses on a continuum) or dichotomous (yes or no response options). Internal consistency (the degree of the interrelatedness among the items; Mokkink et al., 2013) was assessed and the Cronbach’s alpha (α) value was recorded for each scale if provided in the validation study. Reported temporal stability, or test-retest reliability, measuring the stability of the responses to items over time was assessed and were recorded for each scale, as well. Content domains were often identified by using the factors listed by the author indicated through factor analysis of their scale’s items. The content domains often reflected similar criteria used to assess disorders or conditions claimed by the researchers to be similar in nature to the problematic behavior being assessed. The criterion-related validity (the degree to which the scores of the instrument are an adequate reflection of a “gold standard;” Mokkink et al., 2013) of each scale was identified by assessing the scales and criteria used by the researchers to validate their instruments. Finally, the purported construct being measured by each scale was typically identified by evaluating the title assigned to the scale by the researchers and their description of the purpose of developing the scale.
The process for the identification and selection of the problematic smartphone use scales is displayed in the flow diagram (see Figure 1 ). The combined search strategy using PsycINFO and MEDLINE Complete databases and the search terms displayed in Appendix A yielded 2452 potentially relevant articles. From them, 379 duplicate articles were excluded leaving 2073 remaining articles identified as being unique. By screening the titles of the articles, 1567 articles were excluded leaving 506 articles identified as being potentially relevant. Next, through an abstract screening process, a single, broad exclusion criteria was utilized to evaluate article relevance and inclusion. Articles were eliminated if there was no mention of either development and/or validation or utilization of assessment tools examining use of smartphone or mobile phone devices in their abstract. For example, articles were eliminated if researchers utilized smartphone devices to administer assessments of unrelated constructs (e.g., depression, anxiety). This resulted in the removal of 40 articles. The remaining 466 articles were identified as being eligible studies requiring further examination in order to identify applicable measures. Finally, through an in-depth examination process, 78 total scales were identified as being unique and relevant. These scales are organized by purpose and can be found in Table 1 (Problematic Smartphone Use Measurement Scales; 70 scales), Table 2 (Smartphone Use Frequency Scales; 3 scales), and Table 3 (Smartphone Use Motivations and Attitudes Scales; 6 scales), with one scale (MTUAS; Rosen et al., 2013) appearing in both Tables 2 , ,3 3 due to overlapping constructs being measured.
Study flow diagram showing review process on measures of problematic smartphone use.
Problematic smartphone use measurement scales.
Title | Abbrev. | Author(s) | Items | Item format | Item scale | Content domains | Internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) | Temporal stability (Test-Retest) | Sample/Norms (Age: M, SD) | Purported construct | Criterion-Related validity |
Cellular Phone Dependence Questionnaire | CPDQ | Toda et al. (2004) | 20 | Likert scale | 0–3 | Unknown | 0.86 | N/A | 168 female university students (21.7 ± 2.6) | Dependence | Unreported |
Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale | MPPUS | Bianchi and Phillips (2005) | 27 | Likert scale | 1–10 | Tolerance; Escape from other problems; Withdrawal; Craving; Negative life consequences | 0.93 | N/A | 195 adult mobile-phone users (36.1 ± 12.4) | Problematic use | MMPI-2 Addiction Potential Scale (APS; Weed et al., 1992) |
Self-Perception of Text-Message Dependency Scale | STDS | Igarashi et al. (2005) | 15 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Perception of emotional reaction; Excessive use; Relationship maintenance | Unreported | N/A | 248 Japanese undergraduate students (Unreported) | Dependence | Unreported |
Cell Phone Overuse Scale | COS | Jenaro et al. (2007) | 23 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Preoccupation; Tolerance; Lack of control; Withdrawal; Escape; Deception; Life dysfunction | 0.87 | N/A | 337 Spanish college students (21.6 ± 2.5) | Excessive use | DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling |
SMS Problem Use Diagnostic Questionnaire | SMS-PUDQ | Rutland et al. (2007) | 8 | Dichotomous items | Yes/No | Relapse; Withdrawal; Interpersonal conflict; Mood modification; Salience: Preoccupation; Tolerance; Salience: Compulsivity | 0.84 and 0.87 | N/A | 78 United States college students (20.7 ± unreported) | Compulsive use of SMS | Internet addiction |
Problematic Mobile Phone Use Questionnaire | PMPUQ | Billieux et al. (2008) | 30 | Likert scale (plus 1 dichotomous) | 1–4 | Prohibited use; Dangerous use; Dependence; Financial problems | 0.65–0.85 | N/A | 339 French-speaking young adults (25.8 ± 4.0) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Instant Messaging Technology Addiction | IMAT | Ehrenberg et al. (2008) | 3 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Salience; Loss of control; Withdrawal | 0.69 | N/A | 200 undergraduate students (19.1 ± 1.8) | Instant Messaging addiction | Unreported |
Excessive Cellular Phone Use Survey | ECPUS | Ha et al. (2008) | 20 | Unreported | Control difficulty; Persistent need for connection; Specific communication patterns | 0.87 | N/A | 595 Korean high school students (15.9 ± 0.8) | Excessive use | Internet addiction | |
Mobile Phone Addiction Index | MPAI | Leung (2008) | 17 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Inability to control craving; Feeling anxious and lost; Withdrawal/escape; Productivity loss | 0.86 | N/A | 402 Chinese teenagers (16.9 ± unreported) | Addiction | DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling; Internet addiction; Existing measurement scales of problematic phone use |
Questionnaire of Experiences Related to the Cell (Cuestionario de Experiencias Relacionadas con el Movil) | CERM | Beranuy et al. (2009) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Conflicts (related to mobile phone abuse); Problems (due to communicative/emotional use) | 0.81 | N/A | 1879 secondary and undergraduate students (15.5 ± 2.4) | Addiction | DSM-IV criteria for substance abuse and pathological gambling |
Cell-Phone Addiction Scale for Korean Adolescents | CPAS | Koo (2009) | 20 | Likert scale | Withdrawal/tolerance; Life dysfunction; Compulsion/persistence | 0.92 | N/A | 577 Korean adolescents (Unreported) | Addiction | Unreported | |
Cell-Phone Addiction Assessment Questionnaire | KBUTK | Pawłowska and Potembska (2009) | 33 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Salience; Tolerance; Withdrawal; Relapse | 0.91 | N/A | Adolescent and undergraduate students (Unknown) | Addiction | DSM criteria for pathological gambling |
Problem Cellular Phone Use Questionnaire | PCPU-Q | Yen et al. (2009) | 12 | Dichotomous items | Yes/No | Tolerance; Withdrawal; Negative life consequences; Lack of control | 0.85 | 0.41 - 0.78 | 10,191 adolescents in Southern Taiwan (14.6 ± 1.8) | Problematic use | DSM-IV-TR criteria for substance use dependence |
Questionnaire to Detect New Addictions (Cuestionario de Deteccion de Nuevas Adicciones) | DENA | Labrador Encinas and Villadangos González (2010) | 12 | Likert scale | 0–3 | Internet; Video games; Cybercenters; Mobile phone; TV | N/A | N/A | 1710 adolescents in Madrid (14.0 ± 1.4) | Addiction | DSM-IV-TR criteria for Substance Abuse disorders |
Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire | MPIQ | Walsh et al. (2010) | 8 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Salience (cognitive/behavioral); Conflict (interpersonal/activities); Relief/euphoria; Loss of control/tolerance; Withdrawal; Relapse and reinstatement | N/A | N/A | 946 Australian teenagers and young adults (18.3 ± 2.6) | Involvement | Components model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005) |
Mobile Addiction Test | MAT | Martinotti et al. (2011) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–3 | Unreported | N/A | N/A | 2794 Italian high school students (Unreported) | Addiction | Gambling addiction; Compulsive buying; Internet addiction; Work addiction; Exercise addiction |
Smartphone Addiction Proneness Scale | SAPS | Kim et al. (2014) | 15 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Disturbance of adaptive functions; Virtual life orientation; Withdrawal; Tolerance | 0.81 | N/A | 795 South Korean adolescents (Unreported) | Adolescent addiction risk | Internet addiction; Mental health problems |
Test of Mobile Dependence | TMD | Chóliz (2012) | 22 | Likert scale | 0–4 | Abstinence; Lack Control/Problems; Tolerance/Interference | 0.94 | N/A | 2,486 Spanish adolescents (Unreported) | Dependence | DSM-IV-TR definition of the concept of dependence |
Text Messaging Gratification Scale | TMG | Grellhesl and Punyanunt-Carter (2012) | 47 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Immediate access and mobility; Relaxation/Escape; Entertainment; Information seeking/Coordination; Socialization and affection; Status | 0.86 | N/A | 513 undergraduate students (Unreported) | Gratification with SMS | Uses and Gratification (U and G) Theory (Ruggiero, 2000); Individual needs (Flanagin, 2005) |
Problematic Mobile Phone Use Scale | PMPUS | Güzeller and Coñguner (2012) | 18 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Interference with negative effect; Compulsion/persistence; Withdrawal/tolerance | 0.76–0.83 | N/A | 950 Turkish high school students (16.1 ± 0.9) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Mobile Phone Addiction Scale | MPAS | Hong et al. (2012) | 11 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Time Management and its Problems; Academic Problems in School and its Influence; Reality Substitute | 0.86 | N/A | 269 Taiwanese female undergraduate students (Unreported) | Addiction | Internet addiction |
Smartphone Addiction Inventory | SAI | Kang and Park (2012) | 23 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Preoccupation; Daily-life disturbance; Withdrawal; Overuse; Cyber-oriented relationships | 0.86 | N/A | 201 Korean university students (Unknown) | Addiction | Unreported |
Mobile Phone Addiction Tendency Scale | MPATS | Xiong et al. (2012) | 16 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Withdrawal symptoms; Salience; Social Comfort; Mood changes | 0.83 | 0.91 | 641 undergraduate students (Unknown) | Addiction | Internet addiction |
Smartphone Addiction Scale - Short Version | SAS-SV | Kwon et al. (2013a) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Daily-life disturbance; Positive anticipation; Withdrawal; Cyberspace-oriented relationship; Overuse; Tolerance | 0.91 | N/A | 540 Korean adolescents (14.5 ± 0.5) | Addiction | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use; Internet addiction |
Smartphone Addiction Scale | SAS | Kwon et al. (2013b) | 33 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Daily-life disturbance; Positive anticipation; Withdrawal; Cyberspace-oriented relationship; Overuse; Tolerance | 0.97 | N/A | 197 Korean adults (26.1 ± 6.0) | Addiction | Internet addiction; DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence and abuse diagnosis |
Problematic Use of Mobile Phones Scale | PUMP | Merlo et al. (2013) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Tolerance; Withdrawal; Longer time than intended; Great deal of time spent using; Craving; Activities given up/reduced; Use despite physical/psychological problems; Failure to fulfill role obligations; Use in hazardous situations; Use despite social/interpersonal problems | 0.94 | N/A | 244 United States adults (29.8 ± 14.1) | Problematic use | DSM-IV criteria for substance abuse; Internet addiction |
Self-Rating Questionnaire for Adolescent Problematic Mobile Phone Use | SQAPMPU | Tao et al. (2013) | 13 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Withdrawal symptoms; Craving; Physical and mental health status | 0.87 | N/A | 2376 Chinese undergraduate students (Unreported) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Smartphone Addiction Questionnaire | SPAQ | Al-Barashdi et al. (2014) | 39 | Unreported | Disregard of harmful consequences; Preoccupation; Inability to control craving; Productivity loss; Feeling anxious and lost | 0.76 | 0.66 | 140 Sultan Qaboos University undergraduate students (Unreported) | Addiction | Existing smartphone addiction; Casey’s (2012) five-factor smartphone addiction profile | |
Mobile Phone Use Questionnaire | MP-UQ | King et al. (2014) | 29 | Dichotomous items | Yes/No | Unreported | N/A | N/A | 50 patients with panic disorder; 70 control volunteers (43 ± unreported) (35 ± unreported) | Nomophobia | Unreported |
Smartphone Addiction Inventory | SPAI | Lin et al. (2014) | 26 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Compulsive behavior; Functional impairment; Withdrawal; Tolerance | 0.94 | 0.74 - 0.91 | 283 Engineering students from Northern Taiwan (22.9 ± 2.0) | Addiction | Internet addiction |
Manolis/Roberts Cell-Phone Addiction Scale | MRCPAS | Roberts et al. (2014) | 4 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Withdrawal; More time than expected; Tolerance | 0.87 | N/A | 188 Texas undergraduate students (21 ± unreported) | Addiction | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Mobile Internet Usage Index | MIUI | Shin (2014) | 19 | Dichotomous items | Yes/No | Excessive use; Neglect of work and social life; Lack of self-control; Use of mobile internet for other reasons than calling | N/A | N/A | Unreported (Unreported) | Dependence | Internet addiction; Existing smartphone addiction measurement scales |
Adapted Cell Phone Addiction Test | ACPAT | Smetaniuk (2014) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Preoccupation (salience); Excessive use; Neglecting Work/Social Life; Anticipation; Lack of control | 0.93–0.96 | N/A | 301 United States college students; 362 United States working adults (21 ± unreported) (32 ± unreported) | Addiction | Internet addiction |
Adapted Mobile Phone Use Habits | AMPUH | Smetaniuk (2014) | 10 | Dichotomous items | Yes/No | Salience; Mood modification; Relapse; Withdrawal; Escapism/Dysphoric relief; Tolerance; Cognitive Distortion; Resort to antisocial behavior; Conflict/Loss; Desperation | 0.75 | N/A | 301 United States college students; 362 United States working adults (21 ± unreported) (32 ± unreported) | Symptoms relative to addictive behavior | DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling |
Smartphone Addiction Scale for College Students | SAS-C | Su et al. (2014) | 22 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Withdrawal behavior; Salience behavior; Social comfort; Negative effects; Use of application; Renewal of application | 0.44–0.88 | 0.93 | 243 Chinese college students (unreported) | Addiction | Unreported |
Unnamed Nursing Smartphone Addiction Scale | Cho and Lee (2015) | 18 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Withdrawal; Tolerance; Interference with daily routines; Positive expectations | 0.9 | N/A | 428 nursing clinical practicum students (Unknown) | Addiction | Internet addiction | |
Mobile Phone Interference in Life | MPIL | David et al. (2015) | 4 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Longer time than intended; Life dysfunction; Loss of control; Loss of productivity | 0.81 | N/A | 992 undergraduate students (19.7 ± 1.9) | Life interference | Unreported |
Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale - Short Version | MPPUS-10 | Foerster et al. (2015) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–10 | Tolerance; Escape from other problems; Withdrawal; Craving; Negative life consequences | 0.85 | 0.40 | 412 Swiss adolescents (14 ± unreported) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for addiction and substance abuse |
Phubbing Scale | PS | Karadağ et al. (2015) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Communication disturbance; Phone obsession | 0.85–0.87 | N/A | 401 Turkish university students (21.9 ± unreported) | Phubbing | Focus group interviews |
Smartphone Addiction Measurement Instrument | SAMI | Tossell et al. (2015) | 15 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Unreported | Unreported | N/A | 34 United States undergraduate students (Unreported) | Addiction | Internet addiction; Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Problematic Smartphone Use Scale - Revised | PSUS-R | Valderrama (2014) | 19 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Salience; Conflict; Tolerance; Withdrawal; Relapse | 0.94 | N/A | 182 United States adults (Unreported) | Problematic use | Components model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005) |
Nomophobia Questionnaire | NMP-Q | Yildirim and Correia (2015) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Not being able to communicate; Losing connectedness; Unable to access information; Giving up convenience | 0.95 | N/A | 301 United States college students (20 ± unreported) | Nomophobia | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use |
Untitled Smartphone Addiction Scale | Aljomaa et al. (2016) | 80 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Overuse of smartphones; Technological dimensions; Psychological-social dimension; Preoccupation with smartphones; Health dimensions | 0.97 | 0.89–0.92 | 416 Saudi Arabian university students (Unreported) | Addiction | DSM-IV definition of addiction; Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use | |
Test of Mobile Dependence - Brief | TMDbrief | Chóliz et al. (2016) | 12 | Likert scale | 0–4 | Abstinence; Abuse/interference with activities; Tolerance; Lack of control | 0.88 | N/A | 2028 young adults from Southern and Northwest Europe, South America, India, Pakistan and Mesoamerica (Unreported) | Dependence | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use |
Brief Smartphone Addiction Scale | BSAS | Csibi et al. (2016) | 6 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Salience; Mood modification; Tolerance; Withdrawal; Conflict; Relapse | 0.82 | N/A | 441 Hungarian adolescents (13.4 ± 2.2) | Addiction | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use; Components model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005) |
Mobile Addiction Scale | MAS | Fidan (2016) | 21 | Unreported | Salience; Tolerance; Withdrawal; Relapse; Conflict | 0.91 | N/A | 284 participants from Turkey (Unreported) | Addiction | Components model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005); Mobile Internet tendencies | |
Mobile Attachment Scale | MAS | Konok et al. (2016) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Phone proximity seeking; Need for contact; Preference for mobile communication | 0.77 | N/A | 142 Hungarian young adults (Unreported) | Attachment-like features of usage | Adult Attachment Scale (AAS; Collins and Read, 1990) |
Problematic Mobile Phone Use Scale | PMPUS | Pamuk and Atli (2016) | 26 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Deprivation; Adverse outcomes; Control problem; Interaction avoidance | 0.92 (EFA); 0.93 (CFA) | 0.85 | 725 college students in Turkey (20.7 ± 0.1) | Problematic use | DSM-5 criteria for SUD and IGD; Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use |
Partner Phubbing (Pphubbing) Scale | PPS | Roberts and David (2016) | 9 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Unreported | 0.92 | N/A | 308 United States adults (unreported) | Partner phubbing | Personal involvement measure; Relationship satisfaction |
Estonian Smartphone Addiction Proneness Scale | E-SAPS 18 | Rozgonjuk et al. (2016) | 18 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Daily-life disturbance; Cyberspace-oriented relationships; Positive anticipation; Withdrawal and Overuse; Importance; Physical symptoms | 0.87 | N/A | 767 Estonian adults (26.1 ± 6.7) | Addiction proneness | Internet addiction; Existing smartphone addiction measurement scales |
Young Adult Attachment to Phone Scale | YAPS | Trub and Barbot (2016) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Refuge (safe with the phone/uncomfortable upon separation); Burden (relief upon separation) | 0.94 | N/A | 955 United States young adults (23.6 ± 2.9) | Attachment | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use; Attachment anxiety/avoidance |
Selfitis Behavior Scale | SBS | Balakrishnan and Griffiths (2017) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Environmental enhancement; Social competition; Attention seeking; Mood modification; Self-confidence; Subjective conformity | 0.876 | N/A | 400 Indian university students (20.9 ± 4.3) | Problematic-self-taking behavior | Focus group interview statements concerning selfitis motivations |
Smartphone Application-Based Addiction Scale | SABAS | Csibi et al. (2017) | 6 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Tolerance; Withdrawal; Salience; Conflict; Loss of control; Mood modification | 0.81 | N/A | 240 English-speaking volunteers (25.4 ± unreported) | Addiction | Sensation seeking and deprivation sensation; Nomophobia; Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use |
Mobile Phone Addiction Craving Scale | MPACS | De-Sola et al. (2017a) | 8 | Likert scale | 1–10 | Urgency to use mobile phone; Anxiety due to unavailability | 0.92 | N/A | 1126 Spanish adult mobile phone users (32.8 ± 11.7) | Craving | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use; State anxiety and impulsivity |
Adolescent Preoccupation with Screens Scale | APSS | Hunter et al. (2017) | 21 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Mood management; Behavioral preoccupation | 0.87–0.91 | N/A | 1967 Australian adolescents (unreported) | Preoccupation | Existing measurement scales for problematic technology use |
Problematic Smartphone Use Scale | PSUS-R | Hussain et al. (2017) | 9 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Preoccupation; Withdrawal; Tolerance; Lack of control; Loss of interest in other activities; Overuse despite problems; Deception; Escape/Relieve mood; Social dysfunction | 0.86 | N/A | 640 adult smartphone users (24.9 ± 8.5) | Problematic use | DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for IGD |
Smartphone Overuse Screening Questionnaire | SOS-Q | Lee et al. (2017) | 28 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Preoccupation; Loss of control; Craving; Insight; Overuse; Neglect of other areas | 0.95 | 0.70 | 158 subjects from community centers for Internet addiction (22.1 ± 7.6) | Overuse | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use; Internet addiction |
Smartphone Addiction Inventory - Short Form | SPAI-SF | Lin et al. (2017) | 10 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Compulsive behavior; functional impairment; Withdrawal; Tolerance | 0.84 | N/A | 268 Engineering students from Northern Taiwan (20.9 ± 1.6) | Addiction | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use; Proposed diagnostic criteria for smartphone addiction |
Mobile Phone Addiction Scale | MPAS | Basu et al. (2018) | 20 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Intense desire; Impaired control; Withdrawal; Tolerance; Decreased interest in alternate pleasures; Harmful use | 0.90 | N/A | 388 Indian medical students (20.5 ± 1.8) | Addiction | Existing measurement scales for problematic use |
Smartphone Overuse Classification Scale | SOCS | Ding et al. (2018) | 24 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Social network app overuse (S-scale); Recreational app overuse (R-scale); Information overload (I-scale) | 0.85 | 0.77–0.88 | 849 Shanghai university students (Unreported) | Overuse | Internet addiction; Symptoms of psychological dependency |
Smartphone Withdrawal Scale | SWS | Eide et al. (2018) | 15 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Depression-anxiety; Craving; Irritability-impatience; Difficulty concentration | 0.88–0.92 | N/A | 127 European adults (25.0 ± 4.5) | Withdrawal | Cigarette Withdrawal Scale (CWS; Etter, 2005) |
Problematic Mobile Phone Use Questionnaire - Revised | PMPUQ-R | Kuss et al. (2018) | 17 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Dependence; Prohibited use; Dangerous use | 0.86 | N/A | 512 United Kingdom young adult smartphone users (25.5 ± unreported) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for problematic phone use; Psychopathology (depression, anxiety, stress, ADHD) |
Problematic Mobile Phone Use Questionnaire – Short Version | PMPUQ-SV | Lopez-Fernandez et al. (2018) | 15 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Dependence; Prohibited use; Dependence | 0.69–0.88 | N/A | 3038 adults from 14 different countries (26.5 ± 9.4) | Problematic use | Existing measurement scales for problematic use |
Questionnaire to Assess Nomophobia | QANIP | Olivencia-Carrion et al. (2018a) | 11 | Unreported | Mobile Phone Abuse; Loss of Control; Negative Consequences; Sleep Interference | 0.80 | N/A | 968 Spanish adults (23.2 ± 7.2) | Nomophobia | Unreported | |
Cuestionario de Abuso del Telefono Movil | ATeMo | Olivencia-Carrion et al. (2018b) | 25 | Likert scale | 0–4 | Craving; Loss of Control; Negative Life Consequences; Withdrawal Syndrome | 0.91 | N/A | 856 Spanish university students (21.1 ± 3.1) | Abuse | Gambling disorder; Substance abuse disorders; Existing measurement scales for problematic phone and Internet use |
MULTICAGE-TIC | Pedrero-Pérez et al. (2018) | 20 | Dichotomous | Yes/No | Problematic use of: Internet, video games, mobile phones, instant messaging, social networks | 0.72–0.93 | N/A | 1276 Spanish-speaking adults (unreported) | Problematic use | MULTICAGE CAD-4 screener for compulsive behaviors (Pedrero-Pérez et al., 2007) | |
Problematic Media Use Measure | PMUM | Domoff et al. (2019) | 27 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Unsuccessful control; Loss of interest; Preoccupation; Psychosocial consequences; Serious problems due to use; Withdrawal; Tolerance; Deception; Escape/Relieve mood | 0.97 | N/A | 291 mothers of children aged 4–11 (Unreported) | Parent-report of adolescent problematic media use | DSM-5 criteria for IGD |
Problematic Media Use Measure - Short Form | PMUM-SF | Domoff et al. (2019) | 9 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Unsuccessful control; Loss of interest; Preoccupation; Psychosocial consequences; Serious problems due to use; Withdrawal; Tolerance; Deception; Escape/Relieve mood | 0.93 | N/A | 632 mothers of children aged 4–11 (40.4 ± 10.0) | Parent-report of adolescent problematic media use | DSM-5 criteria for IGD |
Parental Smartphone Use Management Scale | PSUMS | Hsieh et al. (2019) | 17 | Likert scale | 0–6 | Reactive management; Proactive management; Monitoring | 0.93–0.95 | N/A | 237 parents of adolescents with ADHD (Parents: 43.5 ± 5.9) (Adolescents: 13.7 ± 1.8) | Parent’s self-efficacy | Existing measurement scale for problematic use |
Smartphone Impact Scale | SIS | Pancani et al. (2019) | 26 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Loss of control; Nomophobia; Smartphone-mediated communication; Emotion regulation; Support to romantic relationships; Task support; Awareness of negative impact | 0.74–0.91 (ω) | N/A | 601 Italian adults (29.1 ± 9.3) | Impacts of use | Existing measurement scale for problematic use |
Smartphone use frequency scales.
Title | Abbrev. | Author(s) | Items | Item format | Item scale | Content domains | Internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) | Temporal stability (Test-Retest) | Sample/Norms (Age: M, SD) | Purported construct | Criterion-Related validity |
Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale | MTUAS | Rosen et al. (2013) | 60 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Smartphone usage; Social media usage; Internet searching; E-mailing; Media sharing; Text messaging; Video gaming; Online friendships; Facebook friendships; Phone calling; Watching TV; Positive/Negative attitudes; Tech anxiety/dependence; Attitudes toward task-switching | 0.61–0.97 | N/A | 942 United States adults (30.0 ± 12.5) | Involvement | Internet addiction; Technology-related anxiety; Daily media usage hours |
Smartphone Use Frequency | SUF | Elhai et al. (2016) | 11 | Likert scale | 1–6 | Calling; Messaging; Email; Social networking; Internet; Gaming; Music/podcast; Taking pictures/videos; Watching videos; Reading; Navigation | 0.86 | N/A | 308 North American adults (33.2 ± 10.2) | Usage | Unreported |
Mobile Usage Scale | MUS | Konok et al. (2016) | 6 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Smart mobile phone use; Traditional mobile phone use | 0.71 | N/A | 142 Hungarian young adults (Unreported) | Mobile usage types | Mobile phone use |
Smartphone use motivations and attitudes scales.
Title | Abbrev. | Author(s) | Items | Item format | Item scale | Content domains | Internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) | Temporal stability (Test-Retest) | Sample/Norms (Age: M, SD) | Purported construct | Criterion-Related validity |
Attitudes Toward Cell Phones Questionnaire | ATCPQ | Aoki and Downes (2003) | 40 | Likert scale | 1–7 | Necessity in Modern Times; Cost Efficiency; Safety/Security; Dependency; Negatives; Functionality | 0.81 | N/A | 137 undergraduate students (Unreported) | Attitudes toward phones | Unreported |
Mobile Phone Usage Scale | MPUS | Hooper and Zhou (2007) | 30 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Behaviors: Habitual; Addictive; Mandatory; Voluntary; Dependent; Compulsive | 0.53–0.88 | N/A | 184 undergraduate students (Unreported) | Motivations of usage | Existing measurement scale for problematic phone use |
Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale | MTUAS | Rosen et al. (2013) | 60 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Smartphone usage; Social media usage; Internet searching; E-mailing; Media sharing; Text messaging; Video gaming; Online friendships; Facebook friendships; Phone calling; Watching TV; Positive/Negative attitudes; Tech anxiety/dependence; Attitudes toward task-switching | 0.61–0.97 | N/A | 942 United States adults (30.0 ± 12.5) | Involvement | Internet addiction; Technology-related anxiety; Daily media usage hours |
Gravitating Toward Mobile Phone Scale | GoToMP | Olufadi (2015) | 38 | Likert scale | 1–4 | Boredom; Social connection; Class-related use; Emergency; Addiction; Perceived behavioral control | 0.94 | N/A | 432 Nigerian undergraduate students (21.1 ± 2.1) | Urge to use during lectures | Theory of Consumption Values (TCV; Sheth et al., 1991) |
Process vs Social Smartphone Usage Scale | PSSU | van Deursen et al. (2015) | 12 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Process usage motivations; Social usage motivations | 0.89 | N/A | 386 Dutch adolescents and adults (35.2 ± 14.7) | Motivations | Perceived gratification items (Chua et al., 2012); Uses and Gratification (U and G) Theory (Ruggiero, 2000) |
Mobile Phone Affinity Scale | MPAS | Bock et al. (2016) | 24 | Likert scale | 1–5 | Connectedness; Productivity; Empowerment; Anxious attachment; Addiction; Continuous Use | 0.83 | N/A | 1058 North American adults (32.5 ± 10.3) | Affinity | Anxiety and impulsiveness; Psychological resilience |
Following the development of the first mobile phone use measurement scales (Toda et al., 2004), mobile phone ownership began decreasing as smartphones became increasingly more popular. However, during this transition from mobile phone use to the “smartphone era,” the terms “mobile phone” and “smartphone” were used interchangeably across studies and, often, within studies. Because smartphones have significantly more components and capabilities than mobile phones and the problematic use of the two different forms of technology should be viewed differently, comparisons of published scales should be made in light of the distinctive differences between the two types of technology. The scales are arranged based upon the date that they were published within each of the three tables starting with the first developed scale in 2004 to the most recently published scales in 2019. Thus, it is likely that the more recently developed scales involved specific analyses of smartphone use and behavior as opposed to that of more contemporary mobile phone devices.
Scales included in Table 1 are those that were specifically developed and validated to identify problematic smartphone or mobile phone use or to diagnose individuals with smartphone addiction, overuse, dependency, attachment, etc. [e.g., Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS; Kwon et al., 2013b); Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI; Lin et al., 2014)]. Although the construct being claimed to be measured by each of these individual scales may differ, many of them are similar in their theoretical foundations and even item content. For example, while Kwon et al. (2013b) utilized DSM-IV criteria for substance dependence to develop the item content for the SAS with the goal of assessing “addiction,” Merlo et al. (2013) utilized the same criteria to develop the Problematic Use of Mobile Phones (PUMP) scale. In Table 1 , validated shortened versions of originally validated scales were included in the review if identified in the literature review. Table 1 includes the majority of the scales identified in the review (70 of the 78).
Table 2 , on the other hand, contains three scales. This table includes scales assessing smartphone use frequency as opposed to general problematic smartphone use behavior. It is important to note the differences between these two constructs. As described earlier, smartphone use frequency can be very heterogeneous due to differing motivations and purposes for use (Elhai et al., 2018). Higher frequency of smartphone use may not indicate the presence of problematic smartphone use if, for example, associated life dysfunction is not identified (Billieux, 2012). Scales included in this table include: the Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale (MTUAS; Rosen et al., 2013) which assesses both use of technological devices – including smartphones – and attitudes surrounding technology use (see Table 3 ); the Smartphone Use Frequency (SUF; Elhai et al., 2016) assessing use of smartphone devices in areas such as calling, messaging, emailing, etc.; and the Mobile Usage Scale (MUS; Konok et al., 2016) examining differences in use of smartphones and traditional mobile phones. These scales were included in the review so as to provide researchers with options for examining problematic smartphone use and/or smartphone use frequency.
Finally, additional scales assessing motivations for as well as attitudes surrounding use of smartphone devices are included in Table 3 . For example, the MTUAS (Rosen et al., 2013) is included in both Tables 2 , ,3 3 because, although it assesses frequency of smartphone and technology usage, it also examines attitudes associated with this usage. Additionally, the Process vs Social Smartphone Usage Scale (PSSU; van Deursen et al., 2015) was included in Table 3 due to its examination of motivations for use of smartphone devices. Finally, a third example of a scale included in Table 3 is the Mobile Phone Affinity Scale (MPAS; Bock et al., 2016) which evaluates motivations of smartphone use, including connectedness, productivity, empowerment, etc. It was important to include these six scales in Table 3 because their inclusion further exemplifies the robust nature of the research and development of measurement scales focusing on smartphone use. The inclusion of each of these three domains makes this review a useful tool for researchers studying smartphone use behavior – problematic or otherwise – as well as associated benefits and dysfunction.
Elements of criterion-related validity, content domains, internal consistency, temporal stability, and purported construct were listed or briefly described for each of the scales in Tables 1 – 3 . Because of this, these tables can be used to compare the individual scales. Additionally, in the following analysis, the psychometric properties and conceptual foundations of the scales included in this review will be further dissected. This analysis will help researchers and practitioners alike to consider the psychometric properties and theoretical foundations of potential assessment tools before deciding which scale should be utilized in their research or practice.
The term “addiction” was used frequently when naming many of the problematic smartphone use scales. This is due to the choice of framework and criterion-related validity used when developing and validating the scales. Many scale developers used either the DSM-IV or DSM-5 criteria for substance use to examine criterion-related validity during development. Others chose to use Griffiths’ (2005) components descriptive model of addiction, which includes the following core components: salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal, conflict and relapse. Similarly, Internet addiction was frequently used to establish criterion-related validity. Before the release of the first smartphone, problematic Internet use was being observed, identified, and subsequently labeled as Internet addiction. Addiction scales were quickly developed to assist identifying this behavior such as Young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT or Y-Scale; Young, 1998). Once smartphones were developed and made available to the public, problematic smartphone use similarly became a concern. Many researchers utilized various Internet addiction scales to validate their scales (e.g., SMS-PUDQ; ECPUS; MPAI; SAPS).
One of the final ways that scale developers established criterion-related validity was by utilizing quantified smartphone use as a criterion to determine whether the scales could be used to identify smartphone addiction. However, most of these scale development processes involved self-reported and self-estimated smartphone use. Because they were unable to utilize concrete and exact documentation of participants’ smartphone use time, they relied upon estimation which can be unreliable (Andrews et al., 2015) and, therefore, should not be considered to be a practical or accurate means of validating a scale.
Even if accurate data were being obtained from participants concerning time spent using their phones, there is no established cut-off point that can be used to validate accuracy of a scale in indicating dependency, problematic use or addiction based upon extensiveness of use alone since it has not been determined at what point phone use becomes problematic. It is likely that a cut-off point for quantitative smartphone use may not be feasible. Elhai et al. (2018) explains that smartphone use frequency can be very heterogeneous due to differing motivations and purposes for use. They describe how a high frequency of smartphone use can be functional for some (e.g., productive smartphone use for purposes of work or school) and dysfunctional for others (e.g., excessive gaming and social media use).
Additionally, a significant number of scales described in Tables 1 – 3 relied upon existing measurement scales for problematic phone use in order to establish concurrent validity for the scale they were developing. This is due to the recognized issue of currently not having a gold standard for criterion-related validity for problematic phone use or addiction. However, this is concerning considering the existing assessments used to validate the new scale likely also used problematic criteria to establish criterion-related validity. For example, when developing the Smartphone Impact Scale (SIS), Pancani et al. (2019) included the widely used Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS; Kwon et al., 2013b) in their study to validate the SIS with an Italian adult sample. This could be problematic for two reasons. Firstly, to our knowledge, the SAS has yet to be validated for use with Italian adults as it was developed using a population of Korean adults. Secondly, to our knowledge, the temporal stability of the SAS has yet to be investigated.
Selection of content domains by the researchers in their validation studies stemmed from their conceptual foundation for the scale’s development and their criterion-related validity. For example, regarding the scales in Table 1 , DSM-IV pathological gambling criteria or DSM-5 gambling disorder criteria were used to establish criterion-related validity for seven of the scales (COS, MPAI, CERM, KBUTK, MAT, AMPUH, and ATeMo). Therefore, these scales’ content domains were shown to reflect the diagnostic criteria associated with problematic gambling disorder. The DSM-5 indicates that, to be diagnosed with gambling disorder, an individual must exhibit four or more of the following symptoms: tolerance; withdrawal; lack of control; preoccupation; escape from problems; “chasing” losses; deception; and associated life dysfunction in areas such as relationships, job, education, or finances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Excluding “chasing” losses, these factors were shown to be consistently reflected across those seven scales in terms of their established content domains. DSM-IV, DSM-IV-TR or DSM-5 criteria for SUDs were frequently used to validate these problematic smartphone use scales, and their diagnostic criteria were similarly, reflected in the content domains established in the validation studies (e.g., CERM, PCPU-Q, and SAS).
Internal consistency is the degree of interrelatedness among scale items (Mokkink et al., 2013). This measure of reliability was reported for most of the scales in the form of a Cronbach’s α value. However, despite its importance in scale development, an internal consistency value was not reported for seven of the scales in their validation studies (STDS, DENA, MPIQ, MAT, MP-UQ, MIUI, and SAMI). The Cronbach’s α values that were reported ranged from 0.53 (MPUS) to 0.97 (PMUM, MTUAS, and SAS). Although there is inconsistency in the field regarding at what point Cronbach’s α values should be considered to be adequate or acceptable, acceptable values of alpha have been reported to range from 0.70 to 0.95 (Nunnally and Bernstein, 1994; Bland and Altman, 1997; DeVellis, 2016). Using the lowest value reported as being acceptable or adequate as a cutoff, four of the scales identified in this review (PMPUQ, IMAT, MPUS, and MTUAS) would not meet that standard.
Although internal consistency is important in scale development, most of the scale developers failed to account for temporal stability in guaranteeing reliability. Upon analyzing the psychometric properties of the various scales, it was discovered that only in the scale development of ten scales were test-retest reliability coefficients provided to indicate that the scales have temporal stability. This is a cause for concern because even some of the most frequently used scales have failed to ensure temporal stability in their development (e.g., SAS, NMP-Q, and SABAS).
This review is the first to identify and report the method of development for all problematic smartphone use scales as well as those developed to assess smartphone use frequency, motivations, and attitudes. After conducting a systematic search and identifying all relevant measures, we analyzed the psychometric properties and criterion-related validity of each scale. However, despite identifying 78 validated scales, we were not able to fully determine the most efficient scales for measuring problematic phone usage due to several issues: (1) most of the scales established criterion validity using DSM-IV or DSM-5 criteria for gambling disorder or substance-use disorders, even though there is still considerable controversy over whether problematic smartphone use should be considered an “addiction”; (2) test-retest reliability coefficients were not reported in the development articles for 68 of the 78 scales, and both internal consistency and test-retest reliability were not available for seven of the scales, which causes concern for future analyses that attempt to identify the most efficient scale(s); (3) the gold-standard criteria and cut-off scores for problematic smartphone use has yet to be established; in other words, these scales cannot accurately be compared and contrasted since there is no validated, gold-standard criteria to which they can strive to incorporate. Therefore, we will primarily discuss practical ideas and recommendations for future research.
The addition of gambling disorder to the substance-related and addictive disorders section of the DSM-5 as a non-substance-related addictive disorder has subsequently opened the door for other behaviors to be researched, evaluated, and identified through developed and validated scales. Another example of this would be the behavioral condition known as internet gaming disorder (IGD). While this area of research warrants further study according to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), the proposed criteria for IGD as a behavioral addiction involving the problematic use of video game technology closely resembles the criteria for SUD and are very similar to how researchers are conceptualizing problematic smartphone use (Lin et al., 2016). Further, based on the development methods of the majority of the reviewed scales, many researchers feel as though it could be time to start assessing smartphone use with an addictive framework in mind, arguably with the exception of “tolerance” symptoms (Lin et al., 2016, 2017). This may in part be due to a belief that problematic smartphone use, as well as potentially other problematic behaviors, should be similarly characterized and defined as diagnosable behavioral or non-substance-related addictions. The majority of reviewed scales reflect this viewpoint. The content domain of most scales (see Table 1 ) are related to dependence-related concepts including craving, tolerance, withdrawal, excessive time spent using, and negative life consequences.
Other scales have moved away from this content domain in their development and have attempted to measure more specific and different aspects of problematic smartphone use. For example, The Mobile Phone Involvement Questionnaire (Walsh et al., 2010) and the Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale (Rosen et al., 2013) examine smartphone use involvement through items assessing euphoria, salience, and overall usage. This perhaps reflects the rationale that smartphones may be especially cognitively and behaviorally salient to some, resulting in more usage, but without this usage necessarily being pathological, uncontrollable, or addictive in nature. Such scales perhaps measure the construct of “liking,” or the pleasurable impact of habitual smartphone use, compared to other scales measuring the construct of “wanting,” or the compulsive motivation to engage in smartphone use resulting in negative life consequences. This reflects an important distinction considering the behavioral addiction framework: more and more in today’s society, smartphones are linked with several forms of reward and social value. It makes sense people would “like” smartphones, feel they are important, and use them many times a day. This does not necessarily reflect addiction to them, despite individual’s tendency to self-report this. Some of the reviewed scales perhaps are better conceptualized as a measuring maladaptive smartphone use, rather than addictive use, as endorsements such behaviors perhaps do not rise to the severity levels of addiction (Panova and Carbonell, 2018).
In a similar vein, some scales appear to measure the degree to which individuals report salient emotional connections to their smartphone. The Young Adult Attachment to Phone Scale (Trub and Barbot, 2016) and the Adolescent Preoccupations with Screens Scale (Hunter et al., 2017) share item content related to feelings of safety with and feelings of anxiety when without one’s phone. Such scales measure attachment styles, in that an individual’s mood state can shift depending on the smartphone device’s proximity. The relative convenience of smartphone functions in daily life can mean that feelings of irritation or concern are likely to present when one does not have immediate access to it. Relatedly, scales like the Mobile Attachment Scale (Konok et al., 2016) and the Mobile Phone Affinity Scale (Bock et al., 2016) have items which measure a preference for mobile communication, resulting in strong preferences for having one’s smartphone device instantly accessible. This emotional attachment resulting in dysphoria can mimic addiction withdrawal symptoms in this way. Problematic smartphone use often co-occurs with depression and anxiety as a means of experiential avoidance (Elhai et al., 2017). But, these scales and criteria may simply be reflecting a strong “liking” for the ease of communication to others via calling/texting, can result in different emotional reactions depending on whether the device is accessible or not. Future research should examine how endorsement of particular problematic smartphone use behaviors perhaps better explained by general psychopathology like depression and anxiety, rather than addiction.
Numerous researchers have published scales purportedly assessing “smartphone addiction” or “phone addiction.” However, some researchers feel as though we do not currently have the necessary evidence supported by research to accurately conceptualize smartphone use as having the capability of developing into an addictive behavior. Griffiths (2013) argues that “we are not yet in a position to confirm the existence of a serious and persistent psychopathological addictive disorder related to mobile phone addiction on the basis of population survey data alone” (p. 77). Perhaps this is the reason that a standard cut-off point to determine when smartphone use becomes problematic has yet to be established. Similarly, the Internet addiction framework was frequently used by the developers of several of the reviewed problematic smartphone use scales to establish criterion-related validity. However, Internet addiction is not currently recognized by the DSM-5 as a non-substance related addictive disorder due to the lack of research indicating similarity in manifestation or dysfunction with addictive disorders recognized by the DSM-5.
Additionally, there is a lack of sufficient research investigating how to effectively characterize problematic smartphone use, and it is currently unclear whether “problematic” ought to be defined by the quantity of use, patterns of use, or by the negative consequences or marked distress as a result of usage. If researchers intend to define problematic smartphone use as an “addiction” similar to a substance-use disorder, all three of those criteria, among others (e.g., “recurrent use in situations in which it is physically hazardous” or “continued use despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused by or exacerbated by use”), would need to be present in order to diagnose dysfunctional or problematic smartphone use as an addiction (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This should be reflected in the self-report scales researchers are developing, testing, and validating.
Panova and Carbonell (2018) support Griffiths’ (2013) previously described argument in that they similarly suggest moving away from the addiction framework when considering problematic behaviors such as the problematic use of smartphone or other technological devices. They reference a pattern of weak study designs in the smartphone literature, such as full reliance on correlational studies, or a lack of longitudinal and experimental studies that examine associated cognitive, psychological or behavior dysfunction. They also strongly advocate for the use of terms such as “problematic use” over “addiction” when describing these behaviors. They noted that it is imperative that a research-supported criterion for problematic smartphone use be identified before using officially recognized addictive disorders to establish criterion-related validity.
In addition, there were many fundamental limitations to the development and intended uses of the reviewed scales. For instance, all of the reviewed scales that assessed phone use were self-report, and, therefore, cannot reliably measure actual phone usage. This is a limitation in this particular field of research that needs to be addressed. Further, when developing these new scales, many of the researchers’ hypotheses for creating these scales were that problematic phone use would correlate not with actual use, but, instead, with associated personality traits including self-esteem and impulsivity (e.g., Bianchi and Phillips, 2005; Billieux et al., 2008; Leung, 2008). Future research may aim to develop or modify an existing scale or consider running an experimental study in which they actively measure phone usage among individuals that includes a method to separate “normal” and “problematic” use. Interestingly, global researchers (Monge Roffarello and De Russis, 2019), as well as Google (2019), have recently created smartphone applications (e.g., “Socialize” and “Digital Wellbeing”) that can track phone usage among other features and even provide an intervention for excessive use (e.g., allowing users to set limits for amount of time allotted for specific application usage per week or per day). These applications are excellent examples for researchers to consider using as an alternative to self-report scales in measuring smartphone usage.
In a research setting, these applications would provide investigators with the opportunity to gather objective data on smartphone use from smartphone-using participants following the instruction of having the application downloaded on participants’ phones for a specific period of time. Future research ought to also investigate the effectiveness of these applications as intervention tools for problematic smartphone use. However, to reiterate, due to the heterogeneity of smartphone use frequency (i.e., functional versus dysfunctional) described by Elhai et al. (2018), researchers should recognize that objective smartphone use data collected through use of these applications or other methods is a measure of smartphone use frequency, not necessarily problematic smartphone use. Additionally, necessary steps ought to be taken to safeguard ethical considerations and minimize risks associated with instructing participants to download applications on their smartphone devices with the inherent function of tracking their activity (e.g., protection of privacy).
Secondly, the majority of the development articles for the reviewed scales reported only internal consistency as a means of establishing reliability for their scale. Internal consistency is widely used in scale development, and the coefficient is based off of the interrelatedness of the items within the scale. However, this does not mean that the items as a whole are necessarily related to the intended construct or possess established validity. If we were going to rank scales as the most reliable based solely off of their internal consistency coefficients, the PMUM, MTUAS, and SAS (α = 0.97) would have been at the top of the list. However, researchers such as Thompson (2003) have called the use of internal consistency as the sole measure of reliability “sloppy” and not representative of the quality of the scale. While a higher Cronbach’s alpha may demonstrate the consistency of the items in the measure, the items may not be accurately capturing problematic phone usage. If there had been other reliability and validity statistics offered in the development articles of the reviewed scales, perhaps specific scales could have been recommended with confidence in this review for future use.
Thirdly, there was a large amount of variability in the types of samples studied in these development articles, which makes it difficult to compare the utility of each scale. The following should be interpreted not as limitations, but, instead, as interesting findings about the diverse origin of subjects studied in each scale’s development. For instance, a small percentage (16/84) of the studies were conducted with participants in the United States, with many of the scales having been written in languages besides English. For example, as many as six scales were developed in Chinese (WEUS, PSUMS, MPAI, SAS-C, SQAPMPU, and MPATS), four in Turkish (MAS, PMPUS, PS, and PMPUS), and seven in Korean (ECPUS, CPAS, SAPS, SAI, SAS, SAS-SV, and SOS-Q). Based on research conducted by the Pew Research Center (2018), South Korea has the largest percentage of smartphone owners. Therefore, the large number of scales that have been developed for and within that population is understandable. Yet, there were also several scales that were developed in English-speaking areas outside of the United States, such as in Australia (MMPUS, IMAT, APSS, and MPIQ) and the United Kingdom (PMPUQ-R). All of this information can be viewed in Tables 1 – 3 and Appendix A.
Lastly, the intended use of the reviewed scales varied depending on the theoretical models or criteria upon which they were based. Most of the scales were intended to measure problematic use, addiction, dependence, and excessive use of mobile phones. For instance, Leung (2008), one of the earliest developers of a mobile phone index used to measure “addiction” symptoms demonstrated by mobile users, based her construction of the MPAI off of the idea that adolescents had started excessively using mobile phones during their leisure time as a way of counteracting boredom due to too much time with not enough to do; further, this type of activity, labeled as “leisure boredom,” had been shown to be associated with deviant activity and negative affect. Interestingly, there was a large percentage (38%) of scales purportedly assessing smartphone or mobile phone “addiction,” which is surprising given the aforementioned literature that has been opposed to labeling problematic smartphone use as an “addiction” (Griffiths, 2013; Panova and Carbonell, 2018). Additionally, while several of these scales were developed with the hopes of being used in the future for clinical purposes (e.g., diagnosis of problematic smartphone use), since there is no mention of problematic smartphone use as a disorder an addiction in the DSM-IV or DSM-5 or ICD-11, it seems as though authors must become content with their scales being confined for research purposes only. This further indicates a need for additional research on the conceptualization and demonstrated severity of problematic smartphone use, and whether it should be given consideration for a place in the next edition of the DSM or ICD. Until then, we are unable to recommend the use of a specific scale or specific scales to assess this behavior due to a lack of sufficient research on the construct.
This review is not without limitations. First, the only databases used in the systematic search were PsycINFO (EBSOhost) and MEDLINE Complete (EBSCOhost). PsycINFO was utilized due to being a specialized database that can provide unique search results specific to topics of psychology; additionally, it has been used in several largely cited systematic reviews (Bramer et al., 2017; Elhai et al., 2017). We also utilized the MEDLINE Complete database rather than the PubMed interface due to the convenience of access through EBSOhost. Secondly, many reliability coefficients were not able to be listed due to many of the articles being published solely in a foreign language and, therefore, we were unable to identify and/or interpret the coefficients; the articles being inaccessible; or simply because the coefficients were not reported in the articles. That last point may also be expressed as a limitation of the scales themselves.
Future research must be conducted in order to further identify potential cognitive, neurological, physical, behavioral or social dysfunction related to smartphone use. Currently, no causal relationships between smartphone use and dysfunction in these previously listed areas have been established. Until then, conceptualizing smartphone use in such a way as to assert that the behavior can become problematic or clinical in nature should be done with caution. Additionally, a standard cut-off point at which smartphone use becomes dysfunctional ought to be investigated. With more evidence of causal relationships between smartphone use and dysfunction as well as a more formulated and standardized conceptualization of the behavior, researchers will be able to construct more accurate and specific scales for identifying problematic use.
This review serves as an opportunity to compare and contrast the numerous scales that have been published in the past 13 years and to analyze the psychometric properties of each of the individual scales in order to determine which, if any, of the included scales should be considered to be adequate tools for assessing problematic smartphone use or smartphone addiction. However, it is recommended that further research be conducted to sufficiently conceptualize the behavior and its development, manifestation, and associated dysfunction. In order to best develop tools to assess the behavior, we must first understand smartphone use with an increased focus on contexts, functions, and motivations for use, rather than simply borrowing item criteria from assessment scales of more established substance or behavioral addictions. Currently, there is still much to learn about smartphone use and at what point and for whom the use becomes problematic.
BH and SF conceived the study, interpreted the data, and participated in drafting the manuscript. TR and JS participated in drafting the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
(smartphone[tiab] OR smart phone[tiab] OR cellular phone[tiab] OR cell phone[tiab] OR cell-phone[tiab] OR mobile device[tiab] OR mobile phone[tiab])
(problematic[tiab] OR problem[tiab] OR dependence[tiab] OR dependency[tiab] OR overuse[tiab] OR addiction[tiab] OR nomophobia[tiab] OR attachment[tiab] OR excessive[tiab] OR compulsive[tiab])
(questionnaire[tiab] OR scale[tiab] OR index[tiab] OR test[tiab] OR inventory[tiab] OR index[tiab] OR instrument[tiab] OR assessment[tiab] OR measurement[tiab] OR survey[tiab] OR psychometric ∗ [tiab] OR validation[tiab] OR development[tiab])